Disaster vulnerability by demographics?
First online: 27 May 2020
Ilan
Kelman
Institute
for Risk & Disaster Reduction, University College London, UK
Department
of Global Development and Planning, University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
ilan_kelman@hotmail.com
Ilan
Kelman’s latest book is Disaster
by Choice: How our actions turn natural hazards into catastrophes.
–––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––
DOI: 10.3197/jps.2020.4.2.17
Licensing: This article is Open Access (CC BY 4.0).
How to Cite:
Kelman, I. 2016. 'Disaster vulnerability by demographics?'. The Journal of Population and Sustainability 4(2): 17–30.
https://doi.org/10.3197/jps.2020.4.2.17
–––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––
This article provides a brief
overview of the relationship between disaster vulnerability and demographic
variables. Population numbers and densities are examined along with using a
gender focus as illustrative of individual characteristics. For the most part,
people’s and society’s choices create vulnerabilities based on demographics
rather than specific demographic characteristics inevitably conferring
vulnerability.
Keywords: disaster risk reduction; gender;
population; vulnerability
Defining disaster
The
question “What is a disaster?” is not straightforward to answer, having been
the subject of books as well as debates over synonyms and the use of
terminology (Britton, 1986; Quarantelli, 1998; Perry and Quarantelli, 2005;
Leroy, 2006). Concatenating the academic literature, dictionaries, and
professional glossaries (e.g. UNISDR, 2017), a straightforward suggestion
encompassing these ideas is that a disaster is defined as “A situation
requiring outside support for coping”.
The
key is the element of coping. Many environmental phenomena and processes occur,
from volcanic eruptions to droughts to space weather, but not everyone is
affected by them or is affected in the same way. If a house is flood-resistant,
including being resistant to contaminants in the water such as salt and petrol,
then a family might be able to move back in a few days after the water
subsides, compared to several months for many dwellings without flood
resistance (Lamond et al., 2011). Similarly, dwellings built and maintained to
resist earthquakes might not require evacuation unlike those which collapse and
kill the occupants (Coburn & Spence, 2002).
The
ability of a society to cope with the environment indicates aspects of its
vulnerability. However, a society’s vulnerability is itself the result of
societal processes which set up people, groups within society, and communities
so that they are harmed by environmental activity and nature becomes hazardous.
It is these vulnerabilities which are the fundamental cause of disasters rather
than natural hazards. The phrase “natural disaster” is therefore preferably
avoided as a misnomer, since disasters themselves are not natural and instead
should simply be referred to as “disasters”. Choices are available to better
deal with the environment, but often we live or are forced to live in places
and in ways which subject us to harm (Hewitt, 1983, 1997; Lewis, 1999; Wisner
et al., 2004), such as not having flood-resistant or earthquake-resistant
infrastructure.
Examples
of created vulnerabilities are:
This
article summarises some aspects of this last point, exploring how
vulnerabilities are not inherent to individuals and groups, but people are made
to be vulnerable by their choices or, more commonly, by the choices of others.
Population numbers and
densities
By
definition, a disaster cannot occur without people and society being affected,
with a disaster’s scale defined according to these impacts. How do population
numbers and densities influence the potential effects? Are higher numbers more
worrying? As always, the straightforward answer is “it depends”. It depends on
which numbers are considered and the specific context, with no single figure
providing a complete answer.
Consider
population numbers. The more people who are affected, the worse a disaster is
generally assumed to be. But absolute numbers of people paint only part of the
picture, because proportional numbers also need to be considered (Lewis, 1999).
In
1995, the Caribbean island of Montserrat had a population of approximately
12,000 people when the volcano comprising the island starting erupting,
settling down only in the past few years. During the two decades of eruptions,
most of the island’s infrastructure was destroyed and the entire population
fled their homes, some to rebuild in the island’s north and up to 2/3 of the
population to other countries, with many but not all of them eventually
returning (Pattullo, 2000). On June 25, 1997, at least nineteen people were
killed in pyroclastic flows.
All
Montserratians directly experienced this disaster, with 100% of lives being
upended and possible long-term impacts from continual inhalation of volcanic
ash still to be determined (Baxter et al., 2014). Yet the number of immediate
deaths was small and even the total number of people affected in Montserrat was
less than half the death toll of the 26 December 2003 earthquake in Bam,
southern Iran (Ghafory-Ashtiany, 2004). Comparatively, though, 0.16% of
Montserrat’s population was killed by the volcano in 1997 compared to 0.038% of
Iran’s population killed by the earthquake in 2003—a far higher percentage in
Montserrat.
Examining
absolute numbers makes Montserrat’s disaster appear irrelevant compared to
Bam’s. Examining proportions makes Montserrat’s disaster appear to be far worse
than Bam’s. Both were disasters in their own right, but each had different
characteristics in terms of population numbers affected, meaning that it is not
straightforward to compare them. Neither should necessarily be made out to be a
worse disaster than the other; both were devastating, could have had their
impacts reduced through prevention, and required major responses and
reconstruction. Proportional vulnerability and absolute vulnerability each
provide different but important disaster-related perspectives (Lewis, 1999).
Similar
aspects of “it depends” emerge for population density. One often-heard mantra
is that urbanization worsens disasters due to higher population densities.
Cities expanding means larger population numbers concentrated within the same
agglomeration, augmenting disaster risk and making disasters worse.
The
flipside is that more people are available to assist. High-density urban areas
sometimes have the most experienced, best equipped, and highest concentration
of emergency services (e.g. Reames et al., 2009 for emergency physicians in
Oklahoma), including healthcare facilities, as well as shorter transportation
times to the nearest one (e.g. Fleischman et al., 2011 for paediatrics in
Oregon). Logistics and planning personnel are likely to be dealing with larger
and more closely confined populations in cities, and this is certainly a major
factor in the high infection and death rates in London and New York during the
2020 Covid-19 pandemic. They are also dealing with smaller areas and typically
more options for supply chains, transportation, distribution networks, and
nearby skilled people—although this does not necessarily translate into
improved or easier disaster responses (Kovács and Spens, 2012). Urban areas
without formal or well-maintained roads, as often exist in informal
settlements, are a logistical nightmare for emergency services and supply chains.
Another
disadvantage of cities is that, if multiple hospitals or fire stations are put
out of action by the disaster, then the emergency services will be overloaded
and the non-urban areas in the vicinity are not likely to be able to take up
the slack. In addition, many examples exist of rural areas with better disaster
prevention and response than larger centres; for instance, Johnston (2015)
found that more isolated communities in Fiji had received less disaster aid in
previous cyclones and so were more prepared than their larger counterparts who
had previously received, and therefore expected to receive, external aid.
Moreover, much is contextual: rural rescuers are likely to be more familiar
with isolated mountain rescue than their urban counterparts, while the latter
probably know large building collapse rescue better.
Similarly,
the siting of a city or other settlement can be selected to reduce (or
increase) the possibility of environmental hazards, irrespective of population
numbers and densities. If a large city develops in a country’s least hazardous
location while a village is placed in a canyon subject to flash floods,
rockfalls, avalanches, and more, then the overall disaster risk might be more
for the village, despite the large difference in population-based vulnerability
and depending how hazard and vulnerability are quantified for calculating
disaster risk.
Conversely,
cities tend to be paved over much more than less urban locales, meaning that
rain runs off and pools in low-lying areas, flooding them, rather than being
absorbed by the ground. Green spaces and permeable paved surfaces, though, can
prevent flooding (e.g. Webber et al., 2020 for Melbourne, Australia). Siting,
designing, planning, and landscaping—irrespective of population numbers and
densities—affect many aspects of possible disasters.
Even
for disaster evacuation and sheltering, locations with high population density
can enact swift and effective measures through planning, training, suitable
routes, and sufficient vehicles and organisation (Renne, 2018). People in
non-urban areas have frequently been trapped when a wildfire is burning across
their only escape road or a flood or earthquake severs it—or if information
flow for warnings is inhibited, so residents decide not to evacuate, as
documented for the Philippines and Dominica (Yore and Faure Walker, 2020). The
safety and success of disaster evacuation is determined more by preparation and
readiness than by population numbers and densities.
Some
cities offer a form of evacuation rarely available in non-urban areas: vertical
evacuation up tall buildings. Provided that the building will remain standing
and not be overwhelmed by a hazard, the quickest, safest, and easiest
evacuation in floods, tsunamis, avalanches, and many types of slides, might be
heading to upper floors (Mostafizi et al., 2019). Even for pyroclastic
flows—which are hot, fast ash and gas clouds from some volcanoes—inner rooms in
large buildings might provide survival spaces against the heat and ash which
would be unavailable in smaller structures (Spence et al., 2007). The key is
that the structures need to withstand the environmental forces and energies to
keep people safe, which is not a function of population numbers or densities
(and which is not always easy to know in advance).
Disasters
are certainly influenced by population numbers and densities, yet much emerges
from societal choices on how to deal with the people in a location. We can and
should make choices to prevent disasters, no matter what the population
numbers.
Individual characteristics: a
gender focus
Disasters
are also about individual demographic characteristics—such as age, gender, sex,
sexuality, disabilities, medical conditions, ethnicity, race, caste, religion,
belief systems, education, communication abilities including languages spoken,
livelihoods, and wealth among others—playing roles in how vulnerability is
determined by and for individuals. These variables have a range of dependencies
and the interplay among them produces complex analyses, correlations,
causations, and chains of influence. Detailed work has covered many of these
variables, such as religion (Gaillard and Texier, 2010) and disability
(Bennett, 2020), while others, such as prisoners (Gaillard and Navizet, 2012)
and homeless people (Wisner, 1998), have only received sporadic study.
Combinations are now being more fully explored through intersectionality, based
on Crenshaw (1989), where multiple individual characteristics intersect to
create, augment, or diminish vulnerability.
To
exemplify individual demographic characteristics, this section addresses
vulnerability differences in males and females, meaning sex-differentiated
vulnerability which, in the literature, tends to be termed gender. ‘Sex’ and
‘gender’ are not interchangeable, since they depict characteristics which are
different and the male-female binary division is not how many people regard or
live their gender. Disaster fatality data has tended to be reported through a
division of women/girls and men/boys (Neumayer and Plümper, 2007) with more
thorough approaches starting to be explored (Gaillard et al., 2017). For now,
gender-differentiated vulnerability generally means comparing girls/women and
boys/men, so the phrase is used here, even though ‘sex-based vulnerability’
would be more correct.
For
instance, following the 26 December 2004 Indian Ocean tsunami, fatality data
from villages in Sri Lanka, India, and Indonesia found that female deaths were
consistently higher than male deaths (Oxfam, 2005). When examining why this
difference emerged, the pattern became clear that the reason was
gender-differentiated roles in society, not that women were inherently more
vulnerable to tsunamis than men. As two examples documented in the report
showed, when the tsunami appeared:
These
gender-based roles and the societal separation of the genders creates
gender-based vulnerability leading to gender-differentiated death tolls
(Enarson and Morrow, 1998).
In
many of the tsunami-hit locations and other places around the region, further
similarly artificial factors disadvantage women and girls in dealing with water
hazards, including river and coastal floods. Females are typically not taught
how to swim, are not always allowed to leave their home (such as for
evacuating) without a male relative, are expected to be carers which makes
evacuation harder and slower, wear clothes which inhibit running or swimming
(and they would never remove their clothes to survive), and tend to be more
malnourished and hence physically weaker than men. Such points explain why far
more females than males died in the 1991 cyclone in Chittagong, Bangladesh
across all age groups (Begum, 1993; Chowdhury et al., 1993).
Many
more factors that lead to women and girl’s vulnerabilities become manifest
through examining gender-based data and experiences (e.g. Bates, 2014; Criado
Perez, 2020), but are under-researched. They represent the typical, day-to-day
gender-based marginalisation and the normalisation of gender-based
discrimination and violence which reduces options for education, health, and
initiative, thereby augmenting vulnerability on the basis of gender alone.
Examples are ostracising menstruating women, a legitimate fear of violence and
assault when evacuating or in shelters, the objectification of women’s bodies,
not considering women’s bodies when designing clothes and equipment, and
devaluing the importance of girls for rescue and evacuation. In all these
instances, the vulnerabilities are socially constructed.
The
same occurs for men and boys, with their vulnerability being socially
constructed through expected cultural roles for them (Enarson and Pease, 2018).
More men than women are recorded as dying in floods in the USA (Doocy et al.,
2013) and Australia (Coates, 1999). The reasons are generally attributed to
risk-taking behaviour, such as driving through floodwater and being in
rescue-related professions. More fundamentally, expectations regarding
risk-taking behaviour are typically foisted on men, especially within contexts
of toxic masculinities, hypermasculinity, assumptions of machoism/machismo, and
culturally engrained mantras such as ‘women and children first’ for rescue when
ships sink (Mosher, 1991). Sexual and physical violence against boys and men
occurs and is not often admitted (Zalewski et al., 2018), suggesting that males
could also decide to avoid safe evacuation and sheltering out of fear of being
assaulted.
The
evidence shows that the demographic categorisation of being male or female is
not the causation of gender-differentiated mortality. Women/girls or men/boys
are not intrinsically or genetically less intelligent, less capable of
surviving floods, or more attuned to water than the other. Instead,
gender-based cultural roles are created, leading to gender-based disaster
vulnerabilities and abilities to overcome these vulnerabilities which, in turn,
produce the observed differences in male-female flood and tsunami mortality—and
the same with other hazards and disasters (Neumayer and Plümper, 2007; Kinnvall
and Rydstrom, 2019). Irrespective of females and males having differences in
physiology, they are made by society to have different vulnerabilities due to
cultural, not physiological, constructions.
Disaster by choice
Ultimately,
vulnerability is typically not inherent to certain people, populations, or
subgroups. Instead, vulnerability is created by society, usually by some
population groups for others; that is, individuals and groups are made to be
vulnerable by the choices of others. Even where demographic features do
influence vulnerability directly, we could make choices to reduce this
influence and to reduce vulnerabilities in other ways, showing that “natural
disasters” rarely exist.
Yet
no situation is ever as simple as it appears in a short paragraph: “we could
make choices” is the crux in terms of why people often cannot make choices,
even if they theoretically could (and would). Considering the influence of
population size on disasters, one approach among many is to seek population
stabilisation by reducing the world’s population growth rate to a negative
value in the short-term followed by a growth rate of zero over the long-term,
once a suitable population size is agreed and achieved.
Who
must agree and how will they agree? Who is permitted to make these policy
choices and to enact the subsequent actions, how they are made, and how they
are implemented leads to labyrinthine political entanglements intersecting with
ethics, belief systems, and ideologies (Coole, 2018). The political philosophy
of this decision is particularly troublesome for reaching consensus and
consistency, in terms of balancing how much individuals should have choices
regarding reproduction compared to national governments or international
organisations. Science fiction writers have even speculated about why people
have the right to breed at will, with contraception often government
controlled, rather than contraception being the norm with governmental
permission required to have a child—leading to tortuous ethical consequences of
either approach.
The
difficulties of managing population stabilisation without neglecting all the
other contributors to disaster vulnerability (and to wider social and
environmental challenges) pushes ‘disaster by choice’ into the realm of ‘yes,
but whose choice’? The majority of the world’s population has little prospect
for fully tackling the deep-rooted, systemic structures which make choices for
them while denying their own abilities to choose. The focus on choice, therefore,
deserves critique through examining:
(i)
Similarities and differences among choice, free will, agency, and other notions
(Holton, 2006).
(ii)
The contrasting adages that everyone always has some level or modicum of choice
and that everyone is always highly constrained by the norms, rules, and
regulations governing our opportunities and behaviour (Giddens, 1979, 1984;
Mouzelis, 1995; Stones, 2005).
If
disasters fundamentally come down to choices—namely, someone’s choices—then
much more work is needed to drill down into what choices really are and the
processes by which choices are and should be made, such as when irreconcilable
societal and individual philosophies and values conflict (Baron, 1993; Findlay,
1961).
Nonetheless,
there is so much more which those with power and resources could choose to do
more immediately to avert disasters. Even a comet or asteroid, heading towards
the Earth to generate a cataclysmic explosion threatening all demographic
groups within humanity, would not induce a “natural disaster”. We already have
some space monitoring networks and some readiness to deflect or destroy
dangerous objects, but we have a long way to go to safeguard ourselves fully
(Schmidt, 2019). It is our choice to provide only some surveillance and response
capability, rather than ensuring that we could avert a major impact under all
circumstances. Irrespective, some natural hazards might be unstoppable and
could indeed represent true natural disasters, such as gamma-ray bursts (Palmer
et al., 2005) or supernovae from nearby stars (Wallner et al., 2016), ice ages
due to orbital cycles (Hodell 2019), and basaltic flood volcanic eruptions
(Courtillot and Fluteau, 2014).
Apart
from these extremes, disasters are not natural because we make choices to
create or tackle vulnerabilities, as illustrated by this brief exploration of
demographics. We need to learn more from the successes to change ‘disaster by
choice’ into ‘no disaster by choice’.
Baron,
J., 1993. Morality and Rational Choice.
Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Bates,
L., 2014. Everyday sexism.
London: Simon & Schuster.
Baxter,
P.J., Searl, A.S., Cowie, H.A., Jarvis, D. & Horwell, C.J., 2014.
Evaluating the respiratory health risks of volcanic ash at the eruption of the
Soufrière Hills Volcano, Montserrat, 1995 to 2010. Geological Society of London,
Memoirs, 39, pp.407-425.
Begum,
R., 1993. Women in environmental disasters: the 1991 cyclone in Bangladesh. Gender and Development,
1 (1), pp.34-39.
Bennett,
D., 2020. Five years later: Assessing the four priorities of the Sendai
Framework to increase resiliency among people with disabilities. International Journal of
Disaster Risk Science, 11 (2), forthcoming.
Britton,
N.R., 1986. Developing an understanding of disaster. Journal of Sociology,
22 (2), pp.254-271.
Chowdhury,
A., Mushtaque, R., Bhuyia, A.U., Choudhury, A.Y. & Sen, R., 1993. The
Bangladesh cyclone of 1991: why so many people died. Disasters, 17 (4),
pp.291-304.
Coates,
L., 1999. Flood fatalities in Australia, 1788-1996. Australian Geographer,
30 (3), pp.391-408.
Coburn,
AW. & Spence, R.J.S., 2002. Earthquake
protection. London: John Wiley & Sons.
Coole,
D., 2018. Should we control world
population? Cambridge:
Polity Press.
Courtillot,
V. & Fluteau, F., 2014. A review of the embedded time scales of flood
basalt volcanism with special emphasis on dramatically short magmatic pulses. GSA Special Papers,
505, pp.301-317.
Crenshaw,
K., 1989. Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: a black feminist
critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist
politics. University of Chicago Legal
Forum, 1, pp.139-167.
Criado
Perez, C., 2020. Invisible
women: exposing data bias in a world designed for men. London:
Chatto and Windus.
Doocy,
S., Daniels, A., Murray, S. & Kirsch, T.D., 2013. The human impact of
floods: a historical review of events 1980-2009 and systematic literature
review. PLoS Currents, [e-journal]
5. doi:10.1371/currents.dis.f4deb457904936b07c09daa98ee8171a.
Enarson,
E. & Morrow, B.H., eds., 1998. The
gendered terrain of disaster: through women‘s eyes. Connecticut:
Greenwood Publications.
Enarson,
E. & Pease, B., eds., 2016. Men,
masculinities and disaster. Abingdon: Routledge.
Findlay
J.N., 1961. Values and intentions: a study
in value-theory and philosophy of mind. London: Allen & Unwin.
Fleischman,
R.J., Yarris, L.M., Curry, M.T., Yuen, S.C., Breon, A.R. & Meckler, G.D.,
2011. Pediatric educational needs assessment for urban and rural emergency
medical technicians. Pediatric
Emergency Care, 27 (12), pp.1130-1135.
Gaillard,
JC & Navizet, F., 2012. Prisons, prisoners and disaster. International Journal of
Disaster Risk Reduction, 1, pp.33-43.
Gaillard
JC & Texier, P., 2010. Religions, natural hazards, and disasters: an
introduction. Religion,
40 (2), pp.81-84.
Gaillard,
JC, Sanz, K., Balgos, B.C. & Dalisay, S.N.M., Gorman-Murray, A., Smith, F.,
& Toelupe, V., 2017. Beyond men and women: a critical perspective on gender
and disaster. Disasters,
41 (3), pp.429-447.
Ghafory-Ashtiany,
M., 2004. Editorial summary: Bam earthquake of 05:26:26 Of 26 December 2003,
MS6.5. Journal of Seismology and
Earthquake Engineering, 5-6 (4-1), pp.1-3.
Giddens,
A., 1979. Central problems in social
theory. Basingstoke: Macmillan
Giddens,
A., 1984. The constitution of society.
Cambridge: Polity Press.
Hewitt,
K., ed., 1983. Interpretations
of calamity from the viewpoint of human ecology. London: Allen
& Unwin.
Hewitt,
K., 1997. Regions of risk: a geographical
introduction to disasters. London: Routledge.
Hodell,
D.A., 2019. The smoking gun of the ice ages. Science,
354 (6317), pp.1235-1236.
Holton,
R., 2006. The act of choice. Philosophers’
Imprint, 6 (3), pp.1-15.
Johnston,
I., 2015. Disaster management and climate change adaptation: a remote island
perspective. Disaster
Prevention and Management, 23 (2), pp.123-137.
Kinnvall,
C. & Rydstrom, H., 2019. Climate
hazards, disasters, and gender ramifications. Abingdon: Routledge.
Kovács,
G. & Spens, K.M., 2012. Relief
supply chain management for disasters: humanitarian, aid and emergency
logistics. Hershey, Pennsylvania: IGI Global.
Lamond,
J., Booth, C., Hammond, F. & Proverbs, D., eds., 2011. Flood hazards: impacts and responses
for the built environment. London: CRC Press.
Leroy,
S.A.G., 2006. From natural hazard to environmental catastrophe: past and
present. Quaternary International,
158 (1), pp.4-12.
Lewis,
J., 1999. Development in disaster-prone
places: studies of vulnerability. London: Intermediate Technology
Publications.
Mosher,
D.L., 1991. Macho men, machismo, and sexuality. Annual Review of Sex Research,
2 (1), pp.199-247.
Mostafizi,
A., Wang, H., Cox, D. & Dong, S., 2019. An agent-based vertical evacuation
model for a near-field tsunami: choice behavior, logical shelter locations, and
life safety. International
Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, 34, pp.467-479.
Mouzelis,
N., 1995. Sociological theory: what went wrong? London: Routledge.
Neumayer,
E. & Plümper, T., 2007. The gendered nature of natural disasters: the
impact of catastrophic events on the gender gap in life expectancy, 1981–2002. Annals of the Association of
American Geographers, 97 (3), pp.551-566.
Oxfam,
2005. Oxfam briefing note: the
tsunami’s impact on women. Oxford: Oxfam International.
Palmer,
D.M., Barthelmy, S., Gehrels, N., Kippen, R.M., Cayton, T., Kouveliotou, C.,
Eichler, D., Wijers, R.A.M.J., Woods, P.M., Granot, J., Lyubarsky, Y.E., Ramirez-Ruiz,
E., Barbier, L., Chester, M., Cummings, J., Fenimore, E.E., Finger, M.H.,
Gaensler, B.M., Hullinger, D., Krimm, H., Markwardt, C.B., Nousek, J.A.,
Parsons, A., Patel, S., Sakamoto, T., Sato, G., Suzuki, M. & Tueller, J.,
2005. A Giant γ-ray flare from the magnetar SGR 1806220. Nature, 434,
pp.1107-1109.
Pattullo,
P., 2000. Fire from the mountain: the
tragedy of Montserrat and the betrayal of its people. London:
Constable and Robinson.
Perry,
R. & Quarantelli, E.L., 2005. What
is a disaster? New York:
Xlibris.
Quarantelli,
E.L., 1998. What is a disaster? New York: Routledge.
Reames,
J., Handel, D.A., Al-Assaf, A. & Hedges, J.R., 2009. Rural emergency
medicine: patient volume and training opportunities. The Journal of Emergency
Medicine, 37 (2), pp.172-176.
Renne,
J.L., 2018. Emergency evacuation planning policy for carless and vulnerable
populations in the United States and United Kingdom. International Journal of
Disaster Risk Reduction, 31, pp.1254-1261.
Schmidt,
N., ed., 2019. Planetary
defense: global
collaboration for defending Earth from asteroids and comets.
Switzerland: Springer.
Spence,
R., Kelman, I., Brown, A., Toyos, G., Purser, D. & Baxter, P., 2007.
Residential building and occupant vulnerability to pyroclastic density currents
in explosive eruptions. Natural
Hazards and Earth Systems Sciences, 7 (2), pp.219-230.
Stones,
R., 2005. Structuration theory.
Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
UNISDR,
2017. Terminology on disaster risk
reduction. Geneva: UNISDR (United Nations Office for Disaster Risk
Reduction). https://www.unisdr.org/we/inform/terminology
Wallner,
A., Feige, J. , Kinoshita, N., Paul, M., Fifield, L.K., Golser, R., Honda, M.,
Linnemann, U., Matsuzaki, H., Merchel, S., Rugel, G., Tims, S.G., Steier, P.,
Yamagata, T. & Winkler, S.R., 2016. Recent near-Earth supernovae probed by
global deposition of interstellar radioactive 60Fe. Nature,
532, pp.69-72.
Webber,
J.L., Fletcher, T.D., Cunningham, L., Fu, G., Butler, D. & Burns, M.J.,
2020. Is green infrastructure a viable strategy for managing urban surface
water flooding? Urban
Water Journal, forthcoming.
Wisner,
B., 1998. Marginality and vulnerability: why the homeless of Tokyo don’t
‘count’ in disaster preparations. Applied
Geography, 18 (1), pp.25-33.
Wisner,
B., Blaikie, P., Cannon, T. & Davis, I., 2004. At risk: natural hazards,
people’s vulnerability and disasters, 2nd ed. London: Routledge.
Yore,
R. & Faure Walker, J., 2020. Early warning systems and evacuation: rare and
extreme vs frequent and small‐scale
tropical cyclones in the Philippines and Dominica. Disasters,
forthcoming.
Zalewski,
M., Drumond, P., Prügl, E. & Stern, M., 2018. Sexual violence against men in
global politics. Abingdon: Routledge.